The nature of the relationship between personality traits and political attitudes

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.013Get rights and content

Abstract

Building upon a series of works by Thomas J. Bouchard, Lindon J. Eaves, Hans J. Eysenck and other contemporaries, we present strong evidence that the assumed causal relationship between personality and left–right ideology is too simplistic. We suggest the relationship is not predictive and instead is better understood by dividing the overarching left–right ideological spectrum into more meaningful attitude dimensions. In doing so, we find that Psychoticism is strongly related to conservative positions on Punishment, Religious, and Sex attitudes, whereas Social Desirability is related to liberal positions on the same attitudes. Furthermore, the nature of the covariance between Psychoticism and social attitudes is due to a common genetic influence, while covariance between Social Desirability and these attitudes in females is largely a function of common shared environmental covariance.

Introduction

Psychologists, geneticists, and political scientists have long attempted to identify and explain the associations between personality and political preferences (Adorno et al., 1950, Eysenck, 1954, McCloskey, 1958). Most of these early attempts to link personality traits with political attitudes portrayed conservatism as a pathology and focused on connecting conservatives with negative personality traits (e.g. Adorno et al., 1950, Altemeyer, 1981, Altemeyer, 1996). Such a view is exemplified by McCloskey (1958), who labeled conservatives as:

“…social isolates, … people who think poorly of themselves, who suffer personal disgruntlement and frustration, who are submissive, timid, and wanting in confidence, who lack a clear sense of direction and purpose, who are uncertain about their values, and who are generally bewildered by the alarming task of having to thread their way through a society which seems to them too complex to fathom” (37).

Focusing on the causal connection between negative personality traits and conservatism and positive traits with liberalism continues to dominate research in this area, though the language has become more subtle (see Block and Block, 2006, Jost et al., 2003, Jost et al., 2008). For example, researchers have worked very hard to demonstrate that the positively valued Openness to Experience dimension has a strong and consistent negative relationship with political conservatism (see Gosling et al., 2003, Jost et al., 2009, McCrae, 1996, Mondak and Halperin, 2008, Van Hiel et al., 2000, Van Hiel et al., 2004). Recent work however, has also demonstrated that Neuroticism is positively associated with economic liberalism (Leeson and Heaven, 1999, Riemann et al., 1993), though these findings are often dismissed or downplayed in favor or a more positive view of liberalism and negative view of conservatism.

The connection between personality and politics historically rested on the assumption that causality runs from personality traits to political attitudes. This seems plausible as personality is widely understood as some combination of innate dispositions and personal experiences that, in general, guides behavior in a stable predictive manner (Bouchard, 1994, Cattell, 1957, Eysenck, 1990, Eysenck, 1991, Tellegen et al., 1988, Winter and Barenbaum, 1999). This is not to say behavior is predetermined by personality; rather, environmental circumstances provide the impetus for behaviors, and personality predispositions increase or decrease the probability of behavior only if the action is appropriate for a specific situation (Bandura, 2001, Caprara and Cervone, 2000, Mischel and Shoda, 1998). Political attitudes, on the other hand, have typically been viewed as much more capricious (Converse, 1964). Although a plethora of research on attitudes suggest that they can be quite stable over time, rather than being perceived as personal dispositions, political attitudes are typically portrayed as preferences related to the immediate social environment and are thus perceived as entirely context dependent.

Such a view of attitudes, combined with the widely held assumption that, unlike personality, parental-offspring correlations of attitudes were entirely functions of cultural transmission (e.g., Niemi & Jennings, 1991; for an exception see Martin et al., 1986), reinforced the belief that personality traits were causally prior to social and political attitudes. However, such an assumption was unfounded, as the heritability of social and political attitudes has been established and replicated across populations (Bouchard and McGue, 2003, Eaves et al., 1989, Martin et al., 1986, Waller et al., 1990).

Indeed, the genetic covariation between social attitudes and personality was explored over 30 years ago. Eaves and Eysenck (1974) identified a genetic relationship between personality and conservatism. Typical of a Nature Letter, however, while profound and novel, they offered only brief discussion on why this relationship existed. And, unfortunately, these findings went largely unaddressed for several decades, with the exception of a handful of scholars. Among these, Bouchard, Lykken, McGue, Segal, and Tellegen (1990) continued to pursue this question (e.g., Bouchard, 1997, Bouchard, 2009, Bouchard and McGue, 2003; Waller et al., 1990). This work has led to a paradigm shift in the connection between personality and politics.

Combining Religiousness, Authoritarian dispositions, and traditional Conservatism into a core attitude/world view system, Bouchard (2009, p. 36) drew on other contemporaries (e.g., Saucier, 2000), and synthesized a theory that more accurately integrated attitude dimensions and personality traits. Such a synthesis implies that whatever relationship exists between personality and political attitudes cannot be strictly causal; rather, it seems likely that the relationships among political attitudes and personality traits are in part expressions of the same underlying genetic liability. Verhulst, Hatemi, and Eaves (2009) provided strong support of such a position and offered the first replication of Eaves and Eysenck’s (1974) findings presented some 35 years earlier. Using a large US twin sample (over 6000 pairs), they found that the vast majority of the covariance between Psychoticism and Militaristic/Defense attitudes was due to a common underlying genetic influence.

Building upon a series of works by Bouchard, Eaves, Eysenck and others, we examine the assumptions made in the existing literature regarding the causal connection between personality traits and political attitudes. Specifically, we estimated the genetic and environmental sources of covariation between personality traits and political attitudes and extended the findings of Verhulst et al. (2009) by focusing on specific attitudinal dimensions of political attitudes and personality traits in a very large sample of Australian Twins.

Section snippets

Methods and results

Data were collected from 1988 to 1990 by mailed surveys to two large cohorts of adult Australian twins enrolled in the volunteer Australian Twin Registry. Each participant completed a Health and Lifestyle Questionnaire (HLQ), which contained items on socio-political attitudes, personality traits, and wide variety of health-related and sociodemographic measures (Eaves et al., 1989, Eaves et al., 1999, Martin, 1987, Truett et al., 1994). Using only respondents who completed the political and

Discussion

The results in this paper offer a more fine grained explanation for the relationship between personality traits and social attitudes than that captured by looking only at the Conservatism–Liberalism super factor (ideology). Specifically, ideology was strongly and negatively correlated with Psychoticism and positively correlated with Social Desirability. These same relationships were also seen with the Religious, Sex, and Punishment attitude sub-factors but not with the attitudes toward

References (66)

  • R.A. Altemeyer

    The Authoritarian specter

    (1996)
  • R.A. Altemeyer

    The other authoritarian personality

    Advances in Experimental Social Psychology

    (1998)
  • A. Bandura

    Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective

    Annual Review of Psychology

    (2001)
  • T.J. Bouchard

    Genes, environment, and personality

    Science

    (1994)
  • T.J. Bouchard

    The genetics of personality

    Handbook of psychiatric genetics

    (1997)
  • T.J. Bouchard

    Authoritarianism religiousness and conservatism: Is obedience to authority the explanation for clustering universality and evolution?

  • T.J. Bouchard et al.

    Sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart

    Science

    (1990)
  • T.J. Bouchard et al.

    Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences

    Journal of Neurobiology

    (2003)
  • G.V. Caprara et al.

    Personality: Determinants, dynamics and potentials

    (2000)
  • D.R. Carney et al.

    The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind

    Political Psychology

    (2008)
  • R.B. Cattell

    Personality and motivation structure and measurement

    (1957)
  • J. Cohen

    Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences

    (1988)
  • P.E. Converse

    The nature of belief systems in mass publics

  • M.L. Cooper et al.

    A motivational perspective on risky behaviors: The role of personality and affect regulatory processes

    Journal of Personality

    (2000)
  • P.T. Costa et al.

    Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: The NEO Personality Inventory

    Psychological Assessment

    (1992)
  • R.A. Depue et al.

    Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion

    Behavioral Brain Science

    (1999)
  • A. Eagly et al.

    Psychology of attitudes

    (1993)
  • L.J. Eaves et al.

    Genetics and the development of social attitudes

    Nature

    (1974)
  • L.J. Eaves et al.

    Genes, culture and personality; an empirical approach

    (1989)
  • L.J. Eaves et al.

    Transmission of attitudes toward abortion and gay rights: Parental socialization or parental mate selection?

    Behavior Genetics

    (2008)
  • L.J. Eaves et al.

    Comparing the biological and cultural inheritance of personality and social attitudes in the Virginia 30,000 study of twins and their relatives

    Twin Research

    (1999)
  • H.J. Eysenck

    The psychology of politics

    (1954)
  • H. Eysenck

    Sex and personality

    (1976)
  • Cited by (71)

    • Managerial conservatism and corporate policies

      2021, Journal of Corporate Finance
    • On the genetic basis of political orientation

      2020, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences
    • Attitudes towards risky driving and Dark Triad personality traits in a group of learner drivers

      2018, Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour
      Citation Excerpt :

      Also it seems that these specific attitudes (e.g. driving because of joyriding or driving in order to show off) in the learner drivers sample are also uniquely related to separate personality traits (narcissism and psychoticism in female group and Machiavellianism in male group). Previous literature supports the assumptions that separate personality traits are related to different attitudes in a variety of fields (Gallego & Pardos-Prado, 2014; Verhulst, Hatemi, & Martin, 2010). The present study’s results differ from suggestion of Jakobwitz and Egan (2006), who stated that it is unhelpful to differentiate the elements for the Dark Triad because they closely overlap (the correlations among traits are from .36 to .70), although we found that despite general associations between latent dark personality and risky attitudes, unique contribution of single traits to some specific attitudes has to be taken into account.

    • Exploring political compromise in the new media environment: The interaction effects of social media use and the Big Five personality traits

      2017, Personality and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Personality traits refer to relatively stable psychological characteristics that influence individuals' attitudes and behaviors in predictable ways (Mondak, Hibbing, Canache, Seligson, & Anderson, 2010). Some psychological and political science studies have found that personality traits relate to attitude formation (e.g., Blais & St-Vincent, 2011; Verhulst, Hatemi, & Martin, 2010; Whitley Jr. & Lee, 2000). These results suggest that individual differences in psychological structures could influence people's political attitudes or behaviors (Gerber, Huber, Doherty, & Dowling, 2011).

    • The ideological importance of worldview: An experimental investigation of the moderating effects of dangerous and competitive worldviews on ideological attitudes

      2016, Personality and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Conversely, Jost et al. (2003) model of ideology as motivated social cognition proposes that chronic perceptions of fear, threat, and uncertainty motivate individuals to adopt more conservative ideological attitudes which promote resistance to social change and endorsement of inequality between social groups. In yet another line of research, Verhulst et al. (2010) provided a behavioral-genetic account of ideology which illustrated relations between personality traits (e.g., Psychoticism, Social Desirability, Extraversion, Neuroticism) and ideological attitudes toward religion, sex, out-groups, and punishment, arguing for a more biological basis of ideology. An underlying assumption across these well-established approaches is that ideology is a more-or-less stable construct, and that differences in ideological beliefs constitute dispositional differences between individuals.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text